Review of General Psychology | © 2001 by the Educational Publishing Foundation |
December 2001 Vol. 5,
No. 4, 382-405 |
For personal use only--not for distribution. |
This article explores the cultural construction of fatherhood in America, as well as the consequences of this construction as a motivator for understudying fathers–especially father love–for nearly a century in developmental and family research. It then reviews evidence from 6 categories of empirical studies showing the powerful influence of fathers' love on children's and young adults' social, emotional, and cognitive development and functioning. Much of this evidence suggests that the influence of father love on offspring's development is as great as and occasionally greater than the influence of mother love. Some studies conclude that father love is the sole significant predictor of specific outcomes after controlling for the influence of mother love. Overall, father love appears to be as heavily implicated as mother love in offsprings' psychological well-being and health, as well as in an array of psychological and behavioral problems.
For most people, life's major satisfactions and pain revolve around personal relationships with others ( Duck, 1988 , 1991 ; Rohner, 1994 , 1999 ). For children, the most powerful of these others are parents. 1 A vast literature shows that the quality of personal relationships–especially personal relationships with parents for children–is a major predictor of psychosocial functioning and development for both children and adults. One dramatically important component of the concept "quality of relationship" has to do with warmth, supportiveness, comforting, caring, nurturance, affection, or simply love. In the context of parent—child relationships, we summarize these elements under the construct parental acceptance—rejection or, more broadly, under the rubric of the warmth dimension of parenting ( Rohner, 1986 , 1999 ). Four decades of cross-cultural and intracultural research on issues of parental acceptance—rejection by Rohner (1960 , 1975 , 1986 , 2001 ) show that parents anywhere in the world can express their love or lack of love in any one or a combination of four major ways. Parents can, for example, be warm and affectionate (or cold and unaffectionate), hostile and aggressive, or indifferent and neglecting, or they can engage in undifferentiated rejection. Undifferentiated rejection refers to individuals' affectively charged belief that their parents do (or did) not really care about them, want them, or love them, without necessarily having clear behavioral indicators that the parents are (or were) unaffectionate, aggressive, or neglecting toward them. These and other such related concepts as parental support, nurturance, closeness, and caring–concepts that are often used more or less interchangeably by researchers–are all central elements in the overarching construct of parental acceptance—rejection or, simply, parental love.
Research in every major ethnic group of America ( Rohner, 2001 ), in dozens of nations internationally ( Khaleque & Rohner, in press ; Rohner & Britner, in press ), and with several hundred societies in two major cross-cultural comparative samples ( Rohner, 1975 , 1986 , 2001 ) has shown that children and adults everywhere–regardless of differences in race, language, gender, or culture–appear to respond in the same way when they experience themselves to be loved (accepted) or unloved (rejected) by the people most important to them growing up. The overwhelming bulk of research dealing with parental acceptance—rejection concentrates on mothers' behavior, however. Historically, the possible influence of fathers' behavior has been largely ignored.
This article discusses evidence regarding the relative sparseness of research on fathers, especially on father love. It then explores the cultural construction of fatherhood in America and the consequences of this construction as a principal motivator for overlooking fathers to a large degree for nearly a century of developmental studies. Finally, it discusses growing evidence about and implications of the recent recognition of the powerful influence of fathers' love in child development ( Rohner, 1998 ). Before continuing, we need to specify that this article concentrates on evidence regarding the influence of fathers' love-related behaviors–or simply father love–in relation to the social, emotional, and cognitive development and functioning of children, adolescents, and adult offspring. This emphasis on the importance of father love should not be construed as minimizing the well-documented importance of mother love. Rather, it is intended to emphasize the need to consider the influence of fathers as well as mothers whenever possible. Finally, where relevant, this article also addresses some of the antecedents of the effects of fathers' love, including gender and ethnicity. Other antecedents, including marital quality, separation and divorce, and children's temperament, for example, have been addressed elsewhere ( Booth & Crouter, 1998 ; Lupton & Barclay, 1997 ; Phares, 1996 , 1997 ).
Little is known about parents' actual behavior within American families
before the 1930s, when empirical research on children and families had its
fullest beginnings. Most of what is known about child rearing before that time
comes from such sources as popular magazines, medical and religious books,
journals, and biographies. These texts tended to exhort parents–almost always
mothers–to behave in a particular way. Or they claimed that parents (mothers)
behaved in a particular way without providing evidence that the claim was true.
In addition, some authors made sweeping but undocumented generalizations about
the effects of maternal (but rarely paternal) behavior. Some, for example, went
so far as to place the entire burden of children's well-being in this life and
the next on mothers' shoulders. In 1849
, for instance, Elizabeth Hall wrote in Mother's Assistant magazine:
Yes, mothers, in a certain sense, the destiny of a redeemed world is put into
your hands; it is for you to say, whether your children shall be respectable and
happy here and prepared for a glorious immortality, or whether they shall
dishonor you, and perhaps bring your grey hairs in sorrow to the grave, and sink
down themselves at last to eternal despair! (p. 27) Earlier, in the 1700s, Rousseau had already proclaimed that "mothers love
will cure society's ills" (as cited in Kagan,
1978 , p. 54). For more than 200 years, mother love was generally considered paramount in
child development ( Kagan,
1978 ; Stearns,
1991 ; Stendler,
1950 ; Sunley,
1955 ). Moreover, mothers were assumed to have nearly exclusive daily
responsibility for the care of children. Fathers were seldom mentioned in the
popular press before the mid-1920s ( Atkinson
& Blackwelder, 1993 ), except occasionally as breadwinners,
disciplinarians, teachers, and moral preceptors. Father love was virtually
unrecognized by the media. A dramatic shift occurred with respect to
gender-specific parenting articles in the mid-1920s, however. To illustrate,
Atkinson and Blackwelder reviewed a sample of 1,482 popular magazine articles
from 1900 to 1989. They found that by the mid-1920s gender-nonspecific
"parenting" articles had begun to reach ascendancy over "mothering" articles.
That is, the term parent began to supplant the term mother in most
articles. It is quite possible, though, as noted by Atkinson and Blackwelder,
that as women began entering the labor force in increasing numbers, popular
writers found it more appropriate to use the term parenting when in fact
they really meant mothering . In any case, only 16% of all articles
published dealt explicitly with fathers. This percentage fluctuated little in
the popular press throughout the course of the 20th century. This evidence regarding the rarity of reports about fathers and especially
about father love in the popular press is supported by Ellner
(1973) , who reviewed every article related to child rearing for the first 6
months of 1950, 1960, and 1970 in three family monthly magazines ( Ladies
Home Journal , Good Housekeeping , and Parents Magazine ). Of
the 177 articles reviewed, only 3 dealt with fathers. In these, fathers were
urged to participate in the discipline of their children and in children's sex
education. Beyond that, fathers were viewed as important role models for sons
and as husband models for daughters. None of the articles dealt with father
love. Early professional publications in child development and family studies
reflect similar trends to those found in the popular press. Peterson,
Becker, Hellmer, Shoemaker, and Quay (1959) , for example, reviewed the
professional literature on parent—child relations from 1929 to 1956. They found
about 169 publications dealing with mother—child relationships but only 12 (7%)
dealing with father—child relationships. Eron,
Banta, Walder, and Laulicht (1961) supported this conclusion when they
estimated that about 15 times as many publications dealt with mother—child
relations as with father—child relations. Furthermore, they found that when
information was collected about fathers, it was commonly obtained from men's
wives, not from fathers themselves. Nash
(1965) argued that this tendency to obtain information about fathers from
mothers (or from children) resulted from the researchers' implicit assumption
that fathers themselves were inaccessible because of out-of-home economic
responsibilities. It is difficult to determine from these early reviews of professional
literature what percentage of the few studies that dealt with fathers also
implicated father love in some way. An estimate may be made, however, from the
work of Rohner
and Nielsen (1978) . These authors completed a critical review of the
literature dealing with parental acceptance—rejection from about 1930 through
1976. In their work they found about 600 relevant studies. Of these, 108 (17%)
mentioned fathers, but 70% of the latter references occurred in the 1960s and
1970s, a time when many other researchers were also recognizing fathers as
significant parenting figures. Phares
and Compas (1992) , for example, reviewed every article in eight clinical
and child and adolescent journals from 1984 through 1991. The authors wanted to
ascertain possible gender bias in the reporting of parental influences in child
and adolescent psychopathology. They found that 48% of the articles reviewed
included only mothers, whereas 1% included only fathers. However, 26% of the
studies obtained and analyzed data separately for both mothers and fathers.
These figures are representative of the fact that a virtual revolution has
occurred since the 1960s and 1970s in the recognition of fathers, albeit only
minor recognition of the influence of father love ( Biller,
1974 , 1981
, 1993
; Hanson
& Bozett, 1985 , 1991
; Hewlett,
1992 ; Lamb,
1975 , 1981
, 1986
, 1997
; Mackey,
1996 ; Radin,
1981 ). We turn now to an exploration of the following question: Why has
father love been so understudied for nearly a century in research on
parent—child relations? Though usually unintended and often unrecognized, much of behavioral science
is a value-laden enterprise ( Kaplan,
1964 ; Silverstein
& Auerbach, 1999 ). Research questions that are regarded as appropriate
or sensible at a particular point in time are usually situated within a matrix
of cultural beliefs often widely accepted within the dominant population at
large, but certainly within the scientific community more specifically. The
issue of fatherhood is a case in point. Fatherhood is a cultural construction (
Doherty,
Kouneski, & Erickson, 1998 ), and once formulated it has implications
for the subsequent behavior of those who share the beliefs and assumptions
defining that construction. The issue here is to understand the meanings commonly associated in the
United States with the concepts of father, fatherhood, and fathering and to
recognize the constraints that are implicitly placed on one's behaviors as a
result of accepting these meanings (i.e., cultural construction) as being true.
Of course, to understand fully the cultural construction of fatherhood, one must
also understand its counterpoint, motherhood. And one must also understand that
both constructions are influenced by cultural conceptions of masculinity and
femininity. It is not our intention here, however, to disentangle the maze of
meanings and behavioral implications of all of these conceptions and
relationships, but merely to acknowledge that they exist. For now we want only
to dwell on the cultural meaning of fatherhood in America in relation to
motherhood and to explore the implications of this conception for behavioral
science research on parent—child relationships. For most Americans, the concepts father, fatherhood, and fathering appear to
connote very different domains of behavior and affect from the concepts mother,
motherhood, and mothering. Semantically, the cultural construction of
"fathering," for example, implies nothing about father love as does the
genderized equivalent of mother love that is contained semantically in the
construct "mothering." The word mothering elicits, for many, a warm,
fuzzy, nurtured feeling, whereas the term fathering elicits feelings of
something stronger, colder, harder, and less affectionate ( Rohner,
1995 ). The term father love is not used in everyday discourse, but
mother love is. Even the phrase "father love" sounds strange to some, yet many
feel comforted by "mother love" ( Rohner,
1995 ). Popular literature is filled with references to both mothering and
parenting when referring to caregiving. But the term fathering is almost
never used in this context. When used, the term is typically found in the
context of the question "Who begot whom?" Even the gender-neutral term parent
is often used or interpreted as being synonymous with mother . And we
have found in our own teaching that students–including advanced graduate
students–often misread and even mishear the term paternal as being
parental , which is then sometimes translated as maternal/mother .
In some respects, the conception of fatherhood has shifted dramatically over
the course of the last 300 years of American history ( Lamb,
2000 ). According to E. H. Pleck
and Pleck (1997) , for example, the ideal image of the colonial European
American father in the 1600s and 1700s was that of the stern patriarch. From
1830 to 1900 the ideal image of father was one of the distant breadwinner. From
1900 to 1970 the ideal father was the genial playmate dad and gender role model.
And from the 1970s to today the ideal image of father is said to be one who is a
co-parent, sharing equally with his mate in the care of their children. This
portrayal of the cultural conception of the ideal father is greatly
oversimplified, of course, but it does contain important ideas that have been
widely shared, and it shows how cultural conceptions of fatherhood have shifted
over time. One of the most enduring historical elements defining fatherhood has nothing
to do with rearing children but deals with the assumption that the major role of
fathers within the family is as economic provider: the breadwinner ( La
Rossa, 1997 ). Throughout much of the 20th century and earlier–beginning
with the advent of industrialization in the 19th century–many American men
judged themselves (and were judged by others), judged their personal worth, and
judged their success as husbands and fathers in relation to their ability to
provide economically for their families ( Stearns,
1991 ). Since the 1940s, this essential role has been deemphasized somewhat,
although it has not been altogether abandoned or replaced. The shared ideology
of male breadwinning was used by many researchers over the course of the 20th
century to explain fathers' apparently limited involvement in child care ( Griswold,
1993 ). More important, however, many behavioral scientists prior to the 1960s and
1970s assumed that fathers were relatively unimportant for the healthy
development of their children in any case ( Cabrera,
Tamis-LeMonda, Bradley, Hofferth, & Lamb, 2000 ; Nash,
1965 ; Rapoport,
Rapoport, Strelitz, & Kew, 1977 ). At the very most, fathers were
thought to be peripheral to the job of parenting because children spent most of
their time with their mothers. Some even argued that fathers have no biological
aptitude for child care, though women were said to be genetically endowed for it
( Amato,
1998 ; Belsky,
1998 ; Benson,
1968 ). These conceptions seemed to have led in the popular press and in
many television portrayals during the 1950s and 1960s to an image of the
irrelevant, mindless, ineffectual, sometimes bumbling and incompetent father (
Mackey,
1996 ). Not all literary and visual portrayals of fathers were negative,
however. For example, movies and television series such as Make Room for
Daddy, Father Knows Bes, The Cosby Show , and others portrayed warm, loving,
involved, and competent fathers. Similarly, some 19th- and 20th-century novels
described loving, concerned, and competent fathers. Bob Cratchit in Dickens's
(1843) A Christmas Carol and Atticus Finch in Lee's
(1960) To Kill a Mockingbird are two cases in point. And even though
comic strip fathers were sometimes lampooned in the 20th century as being
bumbling incompetents, La
Rossa, Jaret, Gadgil, and Wynn (2000) also showed that these characters were
frequently portrayed from 1940 to 1999 as being nurturant, supportive, and
capable fathers. The cultural conception of fathers as being a relatively inconsequential
influence in child development has its counterpoint in an assumption about the
primacy of mothers and mother love. We commented earlier on the latter issue.
Now we amplify on the topic by pointing out that virtually all of the
influential theories of child development during the past century accepted the
unproven premise that mothers are most important in child development. For
example, psychoanalytic theory specifically and psychodynamic theories more
generally assume that the mother—child relationship–especially during the first
6 years of life–is crucial for normal child development. Early attachment theory
too focused on the mother—child bond, as did significant portions of learning
theory and cognitive developmental theory ( Lamb,
1975 , 1981
, 1986
; Phares,
1992, 1996
). With this phalanx of distinguished theories converging on a view that was
widely believed in America anyway, few developmental researchers in the first
half of the 20th century thought it essential to inquire directly about fathers'
influence in child development. Shared wisdom asserted that competent and
nurturant mothering was all children really needed for successful cognitive,
social, and emotional development ( Bronstein
& Cowan, 1988 ). Or, as one distinguished researcher wrote in an
anonymous review of this article: "We spent a lot of time studying mothers
because we thought they were important, not because we thought fathers weren't."
Cultural assumptions such as these about fathering are related to widely
shared assumptions about appropriate gender role and gender identity of both men
and women. In this regard, most Americans–men as well as women–seem to have
associated child care with feminine behavior. Until a decade or so ago, many
Americans regarded it as unmasculine for men to spend much time caring for
children, except as a temporary backup and support to the mothers. A common
theoretical assumption found even as recently as the 1970s and 1980s asserted
that fathers' major contribution to child development was indirect, through
their economic and emotional support of mothers ( Biller,
1993 ; Maccoby
& Martin, 1983 ). This premise may be summarized as follows: The most
important contribution a father can make to his children is achieved through
providing for and loving their mother. In summary, the widely held cultural
construction of fatherhood prior to the 1970s (and still held by many today) has
two strands. One deals with fathers' competence as caregivers, and the other
deals with the influence of fathering on child development. The first strand
asserts that fathers are often incompetent and maybe even biologically unsuited
to the job of child rearing. (The maternal counterpoint is that women are
genetically endowed for child care.) The second strand asserts that fathers'
influence in child development is relatively unimportant or at least peripheral
or indirect. (The maternal counterpoint is that mother love and competent
maternal care provide everything that children need for normal, healthy
development.) The effect of internalizing these cultural beliefs as one's own personal
beliefs seems to have led to sometimes unintended and unrecognized but
nonetheless real consequences. The most notable outcome of acting on these
beliefs throughout most of the 20th century was to minimize fathers' presence in
much of mainstream behavioral science research as well as in clinical research (
Phares,
1997 ). Because it was assumed that mothers but not necessarily fathers were
important in child development, researchers tended to study mostly mothers'
behavior. Of course, they found significant effects of maternal behavior that
served to motivate researchers even further to study mothers. But a subtle side
effect of these results also seemed to reinforce researchers' belief that
fathers must not be all that important because mothers were being shown to be so
very important. Moreover, behavioral scientists and clinical practitioners felt
further justified in excluding fathers from their work because mothers spent the
greatest amount of time with children, and therefore it was reasoned that they
must also have the greatest influence on children's development as well as on
treatment outcomes ( Ferholt
& Gurwitt, 1982 ; Phares,
1996 ). Finally, fathers tended to be omitted from research and treatment
models because they were assumed to be inaccessible as a result of their
out-of-home economic responsibilities. The relatively little research that included fathers prior to 1970 dealt with
a variety of issues, but seldom with father love per se. This omission seems to
have been encouraged by the widespread "tendency among both researchers and
theorists to accept without question the assumption that fathers express less
affection and understanding toward children than do mothers" ( Walters
& Stinnett, 1971 , p. 102). Indeed, many behavioral scientists and
clinicians seemed to accept the postulate that fathers' major role in the family
was in the instrumental domain, whereas mothers' major role was in the
expressive—affective domain ( Parsons
& Bales, 1955 ). Acceptance of these assumptions resulted in very
limited research specifically examining the relationship between father love and
child outcomes. Though it was not until the 1960s that researchers began to find with any
regularity that father love was as predictive as mother love of children's
psychological and behavioral adjustment, occasional note was made of this fact
as early as the 1940s. R. W. Lidz
and Lidz (1949) , for example, claimed that faulty paternal influences were
as common as maternal influences in the development of child psychopathology.
Later, T. Lidz,
Parker, and Cornelison (1956) claimed that domineering, sadistic, and
rejecting fathers were more implicated in the etiology of schizophrenia than
were mothers. Peterson
et al. (1959) completed one of the first studies examining the attitudes of
both fathers and mothers and their effects on both disturbed and normal
children. From this research, the authors noted that "contrary to general
assumption and our own original expectation, the attitudes of fathers were found
to be at least as intimately related as the attitudes of mothers to the
occurrence and form of maladjustive tendencies among children" (p. 129). Empirical evidence such as this about the strong influence of father love led
Becker,
Peterson, Hellmer, Shoemaker, and Quay (1959) and Becker
(1960) to make an emphatic call for more systematic study of the role of
fathers in child development. And later, in his classic review of the literature
on parent—child relations, Becker
(1964) wrote, "where both mothers and fathers have been studied, most of the
research has shown the father's influence on the child's behavior to be at least
equal to that of the mother" (p. 204). For the most part, this early evidence
and call for additional research about the possible influence of fathers'
behavior was ignored because the then current cultural ideology continued to
endorse the primacy-of-the-mother doctrine. Nearly a decade later, in a major review of the 1960s parent—child
relationship literature, Walters
and Stinnett (1971) reiterated these observations: Because we have believed that the impact of mothers upon the development of
children is greater than the impact of fathers, we have investigated maternal
impact to a far greater extent than we have examined the impact of fathers.
Yet, much of the evidence of the past decade suggests that the variability of
children's behavior is more closely associated with the type of father one has
than the type of mother [italics added]. (p. 102) Other early sources of evidence about the importance of fathering and father
love also went more or less unheeded because of the cultural bias in America
emphasizing the singular importance of mothers. For example, anthropologists
have documented repeatedly the fact that women the world over tend to be the
major caregivers of children, but they are not the exclusive caregivers ( Mackey,
1996 ; Rohner
& Rohner, 1981 ). Fathers, siblings, grandparents, and others are common
substitutes. As early as 1956
the noted anthropologist, Margaret Mead, concluded that "anthropological
evidence gives no support...to the value of such an accentuation of the tie
between mother and child. On the contrary, cross-cultural studies suggest that
adjustment is most facilitated if the child is cared for by many warm, friendly
people" (pp. 642—643). Fathers often rank among the most significant of these
others. In the same general domain, results from Rohner's
(1975) early research on the worldwide antecedents and correlates of
parental acceptance—rejection also went mostly unheeded. More specifically, in a
cross-cultural comparative study of 101 societies representing a stratified
sample of the known and adequately described sociocultural systems of the world,
Rohner found that children everywhere–across the full range of the world's
economic systems, political systems, household types, and other sociocultural
factors–tend to be accepted by mothers and other major caregivers to a greater
degree in households where fathers are present on a day-to-day basis than in
households where fathers are present less often. Moreover, results of this
research showed that the more important fathers are as socializing agents in
relation to other caregivers such as mothers, siblings, and grandparents, the
greater the warmth children receive from all major caregivers in that society.
In addition, the more time fathers willingly spend tending their offspring in
relation to other caregivers, the more likely children are to be accepted. These
results were replicated but essentially ignored in a subsequent holocultural
study of 186 societies worldwide ( Rohner,
1986 ; Rohner
& Rohner, 1982 ). Gradually, many behavioral scientists began to acknowledge that fathers
should not be overlooked in child development and family studies, and especially
in studies of parental acceptance—rejection. The inception and at times almost
surprised and grudging recognition of this fact began in the 1960s and 1970s
because of the convergence of three sources of influence. First, as we noted
earlier, spotty evidence was already mounting about the importance of fathers
and father love, but behavioral scientists had been able to ignore the evidence
fairly successfully until later. Second, the advent of the feminist movement
with its call for gender equality in the workplace and partner equity in the
home led eventually to a reexamination of the cultural construction of feminine
gender identity. Reexamination of the meaning of femininity soon called for a
reexamination of its counterpoint: masculine gender identity. This interest in gender role redefinition was fueled by the fact that mothers
with children were entering the out-of-home workforce in unprecedented numbers.
In 1960, for example, fewer than 40% of American mothers worked out of the home,
but by 1972 almost 50% were doing so ( Biller,
1993 ). By 1997, 68% of the mothers in the United States with children less
than 18 years old were engaged in full-time or part-time employment ( Child
Trends, 2000 ). As greater numbers of mothers began working, the feminist
movement called for fathers to become more involved in child care and household
tasks ( Biller,
1993 ; Bronstein
& Cowan, 1988 ; Griswold,
1993 ). To be successfully accomplished, these changes required a partial
redefinition of traditional male and female gender roles ( Jain,
Belsky & Crnic, 1996 ; Mackey,
1996 ). This in turn brought the role of fathering and the influence of
father love into sharp research focus ( Maccoby
& Martin, 1983 ). For the first time, some behavioral scientists became
keenly motivated to study fathers directly ( Bronstein
& Cowan, 1988 ; Giveans
& Robinson, 1985 ; Parke,
1985 ). It is not our purpose here to try to fully disentangle the snarled web of
competing social, political, economic, and cultural forces associated with the
feminist movement that eventually politicized fatherhood in America ( Griswold,
1993 ). Here we simply want to recognize that as a direct as well as
indirect result of the feminist movement, many behavioral scientists began to
study fathers and father love directly. And when they did, they found that
fathers are as capable as mothers of being competent and nurturant caregivers (
Bronstein
& Cowan, 1988 ; Silverstein
& Auerbach, 1999 ). They also found that the father—child bond often
parallels the mother—child bond both emotionally and in intensity ( Fox,
Kimmerly, & Schafer, 1991 ; Hanson
& Bozett, 1991 ). A number of the studies reviewed subsequently continued to show, as in
earlier decades, that fathers' influence was as great as mothers' for specific
developmental outcomes. But it was not until the 1990s that the third source of
influence came into full play, causing many behavioral scientists to fully
recognize that fathers should be included in studies dealing with parent—child
relations. This source of influence derived from research results based on
readily accessible, easily used, and powerful multivariate statistical packages.
Use of multivariate statistics, including multiple regression and structural
equation modeling (SEM), allowed investigators to control simultaneously for the
influence of a variety of variables. In doing so, researchers discovered that
father love sometimes explains a unique, independent portion of the variance in
specific child outcomes, over and above the portion of variance explained by
mother love. Indeed, some studies reviewed later found that father love is the
sole significant predictor of specific child outcomes after removing the
influence of mother love. Six categories of empirical studies show the influence of father love on
specific child outcomes: (a) Some studies look exclusively at the influence of
variations of father love without examining the influence of mother love too;
(b) some conclude that father love is equally as important as mother love in
predicting specific child outcomes; (c) some conclude that father love predicts
specific child outcomes better than mother love; (d) some conclude that father
love is the sole significant predictor of specific child outcomes after removing
the influence of mother love; (e) some conclude that father love moderates ( Baron
& Kenny, 1986 ) the influence of mother love on specific child outcomes;
and, finally, (f) some conclude that paternal versus maternal parenting may be
associated with a single outcome or with different outcomes in sons and
daughters. These six categories emerged naturalistically from the growing body
of empirical research dealing with the influence of father love. Three of the
categories (the second through fourth) are especially noteworthy because they
show that father love continues to make a unique and significant contribution to
child outcomes after statistically controling for the influence of mother love.
We briefly review each of these six categories of evidence. Of course, many
other aspects of fathers' behavior, some of which may be closely related to
father love–such as father involvement, father absence, and fathers'
psychological and behavioral state–also influence child development. Here,
though, we focus on the impact of father love (or paternal acceptance—rejection)
per se because, as studies reviewed subsequently show, father love by itself is
implicated in a wide array of developmental issues. These include youths'
psychological adjustment, behavior problems, delinquency, gender role
development, cognitive/academic/intellectual development, achievement, and
social competence. Moreover, father love has also been shown to be associated
with children's and adults' psychological health and sense of well-being. Even
though some of these studies conclude that father love is the sole significant
predictor of specific outcomes–after controlling for the influence of mother
love–none suggest that mother love is unimportant in other contexts. We should
note here that authors of the articles reviewed subsequently use a variety of
terms to discuss different aspects of the father love (paternal
acceptance—rejection) construct. Many of these concepts, such as paternal
warmth, nurturance, support, caring, and affection, are used more or less
interchangeably and synonymously with paternal acceptance—rejection. Because of
this, we generally retain the major terms used by the various authors. Sample
characteristics of the empirical studies cited in this article are provided in
Table 1
. Many studies looking exclusively at the influence of variations in father
love deal with two topics: (a) gender role development ( Fish
& Biller, 1973 ; Green,
1982 ; Huttenen,
1992 ; Millen
& Roll, 1997 ; Musser
& Fleck, 1983 ) and (b) father involvement ( Amato
& Rivera, 1999 ; Biller,
1993 ; K. N. Harris,
Furstenberg, & Marmer, 1998 ; Radin,
1981 ). Studies of gender role development emerged prominently in the 1940s
and continued through the 1970s ( Lamb,
1997 , 2000
). This was a time when fathers were considered to be especially important
as gender role models for sons. Commonly, researchers assessed the masculinity
of fathers and of sons and then correlated the two sets of scores. Many
behavioral scientists were surprised to discover that no consistent results
emerged from this research until they examined the quality of the father—son
relationship. Then they found that when the relationship between masculine
fathers and their sons was warm and loving, the boys were indeed more masculine.
Later, however, researchers found that the masculinity of fathers per se did not
seem to make much difference after all. 2
As summarized by Lamb
(1997) : Boys seemed to conform to the sex-role standards of their culture when their
relationships with their fathers were warm, regardless of how "masculine" the
fathers were, even though warmth and intimacy have traditionally been seen as
feminine characteristics. A similar conclusion was suggested by research on
other aspects of psychosocial adjustment and on achievement: Paternal warmth or
closeness appeared beneficial, whereas paternal masculinity appeared irrelevant.
(p. 9) The second domain in which a substantial amount of research has been done on
the influence of variations in father love deals with father involvement, that
is, with the amount of time that fathers spend with their children (engagement),
the extent to which fathers make themselves available to their children
(accessibility), and the extent to which they take responsibility for their
children's care and welfare (responsibility; Lamb,
Pleck, Chernov, & Levine, 1987 ). Many studies conclude that children
with highly involved fathers, in relation to children with less involved
fathers, tend to be more cognitively and socially competent, less inclined
toward gender stereotyping, more empathic, and psychologically better adjusted (
Biller,
1981 , 1993
; Easterbrooks
& Goldberg, 1984 ; Lamb,
1997 ; J. H. Pleck,
1997 ; Radin,
1981 ; Radin
& Russell, 1983 ; Radin
& Sagi, 1982 ; Radin,
Williams, & Coggins, 1993 ; Reuter
& Biller, 1973 ; E. Williams
& Radin, 1993 ; S. Williams
& Finley, 1997 ). Commonly, these studies investigate both paternal
warmth and paternal involvement and find–using simple correlations–that the two
variables are related to each other and to youth outcomes. It is unclear from these studies whether involvement and warmth make
independent or joint contributions to youth outcomes. Moreover, "caring for"
children is not necessarily the same thing as "caring about" them. Indeed, Lamb
(1997) concluded from his review of studies of paternal involvement that it
was not the simple fact of paternal engagement (i.e., direct interaction with
the child), availability, or responsibility for child care that was associated
with these outcomes. Rather, it appears that the quality of the father—child
relationship made the greatest difference ( Cabrera
et al., 2000 ; Lamb,
1997 ). J. H. Pleck
(1997) reiterated this conclusion when he wrote: The critical question is: How good is the evidence that fathers' amount of
involvement, without taking into account its content and quality, is
consequential for children, mothers, or fathers themselves? The associations
with desirable outcomes found in much research are actually with positive forms
of paternal involvement, not involvement per se. Involvement needs to be
combined with qualitative dimensions of paternal behavior through the concept of
"positive paternal involvement" developed here. (pp. 66—67) Research by Veneziano
and Rohner (1998) supports these conclusions. In a biracial sample of 63
African American and European American children, the authors found from multiple
regression analyses that father involvement by itself was associated with
children's psychological adjustment primarily insofar as it was perceived by
youths to be an expression of paternal warmth (acceptance). These results varied
by ethnicity, however. In the European American families, fathers' loving
acceptance significantly mediated ( Baron
& Kenny, 1986 ) the way offspring experienced their fathers'
involvement. In the African American families, however, father involvement made
no significant contribution to youths' psychological adjustment, but perceived
paternal warmth (acceptance) did. Neither parenting nor youths' psychological
adjustment varied significantly, however, by social class. Further evidence in support of Pleck's conclusion about the importance of
paternal warmth versus levels of paternal involvement is shown in Veneziano's
(1998) cross-cultural comparative, holocultural study. Using multiple
regression analysis, in a sample of 32 societies representing the world's known
and adequately described sociocultural systems, he found that the lack of
paternal warmth–not the amount of time that fathers were involved with children–
predicted young males' interpersonal violence. Finally, Amato and
Gilbreth's (1999) meta-analysis of 63 studies that explored the relationship
between nonresidential fathering and children's well-being showed that paternal
encouragement, support, and closeness were more predictive of youths' social,
emotional, and psychological well-being than the frequency of contact between
children and their nonresident fathers. As we indicated earlier, many of the studies concluding that father love is
as influential as mother love go back to the 1940s. Most of these conclusions,
especially those prior to the 1980s, are drawn from correlational studies in
which the simple correlation between a specific measure of paternal love and a
specific child outcome is as great as or greater than the simple correlation
between the same measure of maternal love and the child outcome. More recently,
however, the 1980s and 1990s saw behavioral scientists use forms of multivariate
analyses that allowed them to conclude that both fathers' and mothers' behaviors
are associated significantly and uniquely with specific child outcomes. The great majority of studies showing that father love is as important as
mother love deal with one or a combination of the following five issues among
children, adolescents, and young adults: (a) personality and psychological
adjustment problems including issues of self-concept/self-esteem, emotional
stability, and aggression ( Amato,
1998 ; Becker,
1960 ; Becker
et al., 1959 ; Buri,
1989 ; Buri,
Louiselle, Misukanis, & Mueller, 1988 ; Buri,
Murphy, Richtsmeier, & Komar, 1992 ; Dekovic
& Meeus, 1997 ; Emmelkamp
& Karsdorp, 1987 ; Fine,
Voydanoff, & Donnelly, 1993 ; Jacobs,
Spilken, & Norman, 1972 ; McPherson,
1974 ; Monkman,
1958 ; Nash,
1965 ; Peppin,
1962 ; Peterson
et al., 1959 ; Peterson,
Becker, Shoemaker, Luria, & Hellmer, 1961 ; Sears,
1970 ; Yamasaki,
1990 ); (b) conduct problems, especially in school ( Becker,
1960 ; DeKlyen,
Biernbaum, Speltz, & Greenberg, 1998 ; DeKlyen,
Speltz, & Greenberg, 1998 ; McPherson,
1974 ; Paley,
Conger, & Harold, 2000 ; Patterson,
Reid, & Dishion, 1992 ; Renk,
Phares, & Epps, 1999 ; Russell
& Russell, 1996 ; Siantz
& Smith, 1994 ); (c) cognitive and academic performance issues ( Amato,
1998 ; Carroll,
1973 ; Easterbrooks
& Goldberg, 1984 ; Heilbrun,
Orr, & Harrell, 1966 ; Peppin,
1962 ); (d) mental illness ( Arrindell,
Emmelkamp, Monsma, & Brilman, 1983 ; Crook,
Raskin, & Eliot, 1981 ; Greenberger
& Chen, 1996 ; R. W. Lidz
& Lidz, 1949 ; T. Lidz et
al., 1956 ; Richter,
Richter, & Eisemann, 1990 ); and (e) substance abuse ( Barnes,
1984 ; Prendergast
& Schaefer, 1974 ). Only newer studies in the 1980s and 1990s used complex multivariate analyses
to systematically test for the relative influence of fathers' love in contrast
to mothers' love. The work of Young,
Miller, Norton, and Hill (1995) illustrates this. These authors drew from a
national sample of 640 adolescents 12 to 16 years old living in two-parent
families. Employing SEM techniques, they found that perceived paternal love and
caring were as predictive of sons' and daughters' life satisfaction–including
their sense of well-being–as maternal love and caring. Review of a broad range
of studies such as these led Lamb
(1997) to conclude that fathers and mothers seem to influence their children in similar rather than
dissimilar ways. Contrary to the expectations of many psychologists, including
myself, who have studied paternal influences on children, the differences
between mothers and fathers appear much less important than the similarities.
Students of socialization have consistently found that parental warmth,
nurturance, and closeness are associated with positive child outcomes whether
the parent or adult involved is a mother or a father. The important dimensions
of parental influence are those that have to do with parental characteristics
rather than gender-related characteristics. (p. 13) Two types of studies are common in this category. First, results of some
bivariate correlational studies have led researchers to conclude that fathers'
love is more strongly associated than mothers' love with specific child
behaviors such as those noted subsequently. Second, the 1980s and especially the
1990s saw a proliferation of studies using multiple regression and SEM. As these
analytic procedures became more commonplace, it also became more common to
discover that the influence of father love explains a unique, independent
portion of the variance in specific child outcomes–detailed subsequently–over
and above the portion of variance explained by mother love. Studies in this category tend to deal with one or more of the following six
issues among children, adolescents, and young adults: (a) personality and
psychological adjustment problems ( Amato,
1994 ; Dominy,
Johnson, & Koch, 2000 ; Komarovsky,
1976 ; Stagner,
1933 ; Tacon
& Caldera, 2001 ), (b) conduct problems ( Chen,
Liu, & Li, 2000 ; Eron et
al., 1961 ; Grant et
al., 2000 ), (c) delinquency ( Andry,
1962 ), (d) mental illness ( Barrera
& Garrison-Jones, 1992 ; Lefkowitz
& Tesiny, 1984 ), (e) substance abuse ( Brook
& Brook, 1988 ; Emmelkamp
& Heeres, 1988 ), and (f) psychological health and well-being ( Amato,
1994 ). We briefly review an example of each issue. Research by Dominy
et al. (2000) illustrates the first issue. In this work, the authors studied
the relation between perceived childhood experiences of parental (maternal and
paternal) acceptance—rejection and the presence or absence of binge eating
disorder (BED) among a group of 113 women. Eighty-three of these women were
obese. Of these, 32 suffered from BED. Thirty women, however, neither were obese
nor had an eating disorder. The authors found that women with BED perceived
their fathers to be significantly more rejecting than did women in either of the
other two groups. In addition, the women with BED perceived their fathers to be
significantly more rejecting than their mothers. A 2-year longitudinal study of 258 sixth-grade students (at Time 1) in the
People's Republic of China serves to illustrate the second issue: dealing with
conduct problems. In this study Chen et
al. (2000) found that paternal but not maternal warmth at Time 1 was
negatively associated with youths' disruptive aggression toward peers at Time 2.
In addition, they found that paternal but not maternal warmth was positively
associated with children's academic performance and social competence as judged
by teachers. An older work by Andry
(1962) illustrates the third type of study: the link between paternal and
maternal rejection and delinquency. Here Andry found in a matched sample of 80
delinquent boys 11 through 15 years of age that the great majority of
delinquents felt rejected by their fathers but not necessarily by their mothers.
Nondelinquents, on the other hand, tended to feel loved by both parents. Of
special interest in Andry's study is the fact that fathers as well as mothers in
the two groups tended to corroborate the youths' perceptions of parenting. Research by Rohner
and Brothers (1999) illustrates the fourth issue, dealing with the link
between parenting and mental illness. These authors examined the relation
between perceived parental (paternal and maternal) acceptance—rejection and
self-reported psychological adjustment in a sample of 17 women diagnosed as
suffering from borderline personality disorder. This group was compared with a
control sample of 18 nonclinical women. Results showed that women in the
clinical group tended to perceive qualitatively more paternal rejection than
acceptance in their families of origin. Perceived maternal rejection was also
elevated somewhat, but not to the point where these women experienced
qualitatively more maternal rejection than acceptance. And, as is generally true
throughout much of America ( Rohner,
1986 , 2001
), most of the women in the nonclinical control group tended to perceive
substantial paternal as well as maternal love and overall acceptance. Research by Campo
and Rohner (1992) illustrates the fifth issue, dealing with the relation
between parenting and substance abuse. These researchers studied 40 drug abusers
in relation to a control sample of 40 nonabusers. On the average, the
polydrug-addicted group–both males and females–had experienced qualitatively
more paternal rejection than acceptance before the onset of serious drug use as
adolescents. The substance abusers had also experienced significant love
withdrawal at the hands of their mothers, but not to the point of having
experienced qualitatively more rejection than acceptance. Participants in the
nonabuse group, on the other hand, had experienced substantial paternal as well
as maternal love and acceptance in their families of origin. Discriminant
function analysis showed that perceived paternal acceptance—rejection,
self-reported psychological adjustment, perceived maternal acceptance—rejection,
and level of education (in that order of importance) predicted with 91.2%
accuracy who among the 80 participants were drug abusers versus nonabusers.
Perceptions of father love and love withdrawal were overwhelmingly the best
single predictor. Finally, Amato's
(1994) work serves to illustrate the sixth issue. Here, Amato showed that
father love is sometimes implicated to a greater extent than mother love in
adult offsprings' overall psychological health and well-being. Drawing from
telephone interviews with a national sample of 471 young adults, he found that
perceived closeness to fathers for both sons and daughters made a unique
contribution–over and above the contribution made by perceived closeness to
mothers–to adult offsprings' happiness, life satisfaction, and low psychological
distress (i.e., overall psychological well-being). In the author's words,
"Regardless of the quality of the mother-child relationship, the closer adult
offspring were to their fathers, the happier, more satisfied, and less
distressed they reported being" (p. 1039). A growing number of studies using a variety of multivariate statistics have
begun to conclude that father love is occasionally the sole significant
predictor of specific child outcomes after removing the influence of mother
love. Studies in this category tend to deal most often with one or more of the
following three issues among children, adolescents, and young adults: (a)
personality and psychological adjustment problems ( Barnett,
Marshall, & Pleck, 1992 ; Bartle,
Anderson, & Sabatelli, 1989 ; Dickie
et al., 1997 ; DuBos,
Eitel, & Felner, 1994 ; Matsuda
& Ritblatt, 1998 ; Wagner
& Philips, 1992 ), (b) conduct and delinquency problems ( Kroupa,
1988 ), and (c) substance abuse ( Brook,
Whiteman, & Gordon, 1981 ; Eldred,
Brown, & Mahabir, 1974 ). Cole and
McPherson (1993) , for example, concluded from their SEM analysis of 107
adolescents and parents that father—child conflict but not mother—child conflict
(each controlling for the other) was positively associated with adolescent
depressive symptoms. Moreover, father—adolescent cohesion was positively
associated with the absence of adolescent depressive symptoms. These results are
consistent with Barrera
and Garrison-Jones's (1992) conclusion that adolescents' satisfaction with
paternal support was related to adolescent depression, whereas maternal support
was not. A similar study by Barnett
et al. (1992) was conducted among 285 married adult sons with two living
parents. When measures of the quality of both mother—son and father—son
relationships were entered simultaneously into a regression equation, only the
father—son relationship was significantly related to adult sons' psychological
distress (a summed measure of anxiety and depression). In a sample of 70 middle- to lower-middle-class adolescents, Forehand
and Nousiainen (1993) assessed relations between school functioning, as
evaluated by teachers, and youths' perceptions of parental (paternal and
maternal) acceptance—rejection. Using stepwise multiple regression, the authors
found that teachers' ratings of youths' social competence and conduct problems
were associated with the adolescents' perceptions of paternal but not maternal
acceptance. A 6-year longitudinal study begun in 1981 by Brody,
Moore, and Glei (1994) showed that paternal (but not maternal) warmth had a
significant long-term effect in shaping adolescents' attitudes in 1987 toward
such social issues as marriage, divorce, sex roles, child support, welfare, and
teenage childbearing. More specifically, the warmer fathers were and the more
adolescents were allowed to participate in family decision making in 1981, the
more adolescents internalized their parents' values over time as being their
own. This relationship was true only for warm father—adolescent relationships.
Warm mother—adolescent relationships had no significant effect on youths' later
attitudes. Results from this study were based on 592 families participating in a
nationally representative household survey of 11- to 16-year-old youths. Finally, Veneziano
(2000) found in a sample of 281 African American and European American
families that only paternal warmth was significantly related to the European
American youths' psychological adjustment when controlling for the influence of
maternal warmth. Indeed, maternal warmth dropped from the regression model
altogether. However, in the African American families, paternal as well as
maternal warmth was significantly related to youths' psychological adjustment,
making both independent and joint contributions. Fathers' behavior may moderate and be moderated by–that is, interact with (
Baron
& Kenny, 1986 ; Hull,
Tedlie, & Zahn, 1992 )–other influences within the family. Apparently,
however, only one study so far has addressed the issue of mother love having
different effects on specific child outcomes depending on the level of father
love. Here, Forehand
and Nousiainen (1993) found in a sample of 70 adolescents and their parents
that the relative level of fathers' love and acceptance moderated the
contribution of mothers' loving acceptance to adolescent functioning.
Specifically, variation in father love contributed to adolescents' cognitive
competence as well as to anxiety—withdrawal (internalizing problems) through its
interaction with maternal acceptance. Regarding cognitive competence, for
example, when mothers were high in acceptance but fathers were low, teachers
judged youths' cognitive competence to be quite low, lower even than when
mothers' acceptance was also low. But when fathers' loving acceptance was high,
mothers' loving acceptance was associated with the most positive levels of
cognitive competence. From this study, the authors drew the important inference
that "simply including fathers in parent-adolescent research is insufficient.
Instead, the potential ways in which each parent's style contributes to the
other parent's style must be considered" (p. 219). Two types of research tend to be found in this category. First, some research
shows that one pattern of paternal behavior and a different pattern of maternal
behavior is associated with a single outcome in sons, daughters, or sometimes
both offspring. Second, other research in this category shows that a single
pattern of paternal love-related behavior is associated with one outcome for
sons and a different outcome for daughters. We briefly review examples of both
types. The work of Barber
and Thomas (1986) illustrates the first type. These authors found in a
sample of 527 adolescents that daughters' self-esteem was best predicted by
fathers' physical affection (kisses and hugs) and by mothers' general support,
including maternal praise, approval, encouragement, use of terms of endearment,
and helping behaviors. Sons' self-esteem, on the other hand, was best predicted
by fathers' sustained contact (e.g., picking up the boy for fun and safety) and
by mothers' companionship (i.e., spending time with the boy and sharing
activities with him). In addition, in perhaps the earliest study ever done of
this type, Fitz-Simmons
(1935) found that paternal rejection and maternal overprotection
characterized the parenting styles among 96 emotionally withdrawn children being
treated in child guidance clinics. Moreover, a substantial literature finds
differences in paternal versus maternal love-related behaviors influencing
different aspects of the gender role development of both boys and girls ( Biller
& Borstelmann, 1967 ; Bronson,
1959 ; Distler,
1965 ; Kelly
& Worell, 1976 ; Mussen,
1961 ; Mussen
& Distler, 1959 ; Orlofsky,
1979 ; Payne
& Mussen, 1956 ). The work of Jordan,
Radin, and Epstein (1975) serves to illustrate the second type of study in
this category, that is, studies concluding that a single pattern of paternal
love-related behavior is associated with different outcomes for sons versus
daughters. These authors found in a sample of 180 European American children 4
years of age that paternal nurturance was positively associated with
middle-class sons' performance on the Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale ( Terman
& Merrill, 1960 ). Paternal nurturance was unrelated, however, to
daughters' performance on the scale. Finally, a second study is also broadly illustrative of this second type of
research. Here Booth
and Amato (1994) found in a longitudinal study of 419 parents and their
adult children that a poor relationship between spouses while children were
living at home was associated 12 years later with somewhat different outcomes
for sons and daughters. Specifically, adult sons felt somewhat less close to
both parents than did sons whose parents had had a good marital relationship.
Daughters, on the other hand, felt much less close to their fathers but only
slightly less close to their mothers. Thus, the authors concluded, the
father—daughter tie tends to be especially vulnerable in the context of serious
marital problems between parents, whereas the mother—daughter tie tends to be
especially resilient. The body of work reviewed in this article shows that paternal
acceptance—rejection (father love) is heavily implicated not only in children's
and adults' psychological well-being and health but also in an array of
psychological and behavioral problems. Moreover, this body of work suggests that
father love may affect offspring development at all ages from infancy through at
least young adulthood. More specifically, evidence discussed here shows that
father love is often associated as robustly as mother love with a variety of
outcomes. For example, both father and mother love—withdrawal (parental
rejection) have been significantly implicated in offsprings' personality and
psychological adjustment problems, including issues of negative self-concept,
negative self-esteem, emotional instability, anxiety, social and emotional
withdrawal, and aggression; conduct problems, including externalizing behaviors
and delinquency; drug and alcohol abuse; cognitive and academic difficulties;
and forms of mental disorder such as depression, depressed affect, and
borderline personality disorder. On the other hand, both paternal and maternal love (parental acceptance) have
been shown to be effective buffers against many of these problems, as well as
being associated with a sense of happiness and well-being in adolescence and
adulthood, physical and psychological health, social competence, academic
achievement, and the internalization of parental values as one's own values.
Even though mother love is associated with all of these outcomes, evidence
reviewed in this article suggests that father love is even more strongly
associated with many. Moreover, multiple regression and SEM analyses conclude
that the influence of mother love sometimes disappears altogether, leaving
father love as the sole significant predictor of such outcomes as personality
and psychological adjustment problems, conduct and delinquency problems, and
substance abuse. Several problems and limitations characterize this body of research. First,
most of the work appears to deal with middle-class European American parents.
Research on the influence of father love among African Americans, Hispanic
Americans, Asian Americans, and Native Americans is relatively uncommon, and it
is equally rare in cross-cultural studies. As a result, it remains for future
research to determine the extent to which results reported here can be
generalized beyond middle-class European American families. Second, many of the
studies reviewed here relied on a single source (e.g., youths' or parents'
reports) to provide information about both parental acceptance—rejection and the
relevant outcome variable(s). As noted by Campbell
and Fiske (1959) ; Marsiglio,
Amato, Day, and Lamb (2000) ; and others, however, shared-method variance in
studies such as these might artificially inflate the correlation between
variables, resulting in an overestimate of the true effect size. Third, even though it seems unmistakably clear that father love makes an
important contribution to offsprings' development and psychological functioning,
it is not at all clear why paternal acceptance—rejection is sometimes more
strongly associated with specific child outcomes than is maternal
acceptance—rejection. And it is unclear why patterns of paternal versus maternal
parenting are sometimes associated with different outcomes for sons, daughters,
or both children. Part of the reason for these differences no doubt lies in the
fact that fathers and mothers often interact with their children in somewhat
different ways ( Fagot,
1995 ). Fathers generally interact with children less frequently than do
mothers, for example, and they generally tend to be less involved in caregiving
( Fagot,
1995 ; K. N.
Harris et al., 1998 ). Moreover, when they do interact with their children,
fathers often initiate different types of behaviors from those of mothers. For
example, fathers tend to engage in more physical, rough-and-tumble, and
idiosyncratic play than do mothers ( Collins
& Russell, 1991 ; Forehand
& Nousiainen, 1993 ; Parke,
1996 ). In addition, fathers are more likely than mothers to encourage
children's competitiveness and independence and to encourage their children to
take risks ( Cabrera
et al., 2000 ). Nothing in these behaviors per se, however, appears to
explain why father love sometimes has different effects from mother love. Possibly, evolutionary and behavior-genetic influences should be considered
in attempts to explain some of these effects ( Collins,
Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000 , 2001
; J. R. Harris,
1998 ; Rowe,
1994 , 2001
). Some of these differences, however, are probably related to the fact that
fathers are often perceived to have more power and authority within the family
than do mothers ( Radin,
1981 ). It remains for future research to inquire directly about the
relevance of these mechanisms. Until then we can know that father love is often
as influential as mother love–and sometimes more so–but we cannot know for sure
why this is true. Proper recognition of the influence of fathers and father love should have
several salutary effects. First, widespread recognition of fathers' influence
may help motivate many men to become more involved in nurturing child care. Simons,
Whitbeck, Conger, and Melby (1990) supported this point when they found that
"fathers are more likely to engage in nurturing activities when they believe
such behaviors will make an important difference in the life of their child" (p.
387). Of course, the degree to which men are likely to become involved in
nurturing paternal behavior is at least partly a function of their mates'
internalized cultural beliefs about the maternal role ( Parke,
1995 ). That is, because of the enormous cultural emphasis placed on the
role of motherhood in America, some women are ambivalent about encouraging
fathers to become heavily involved as nurturing parents ( Allen
& Hawkins, 1999 ; Biller,
1993 ; Cowan
& Bronstein, 1988 ; Doherty
et al., 1998 ; Marsiglio
et al., 2000 ). And of course, as already mentioned, some men have
difficulty redefining masculinity to include nurturing paternal behavior.
Nonetheless, the evidence seems clear that mothers are more effective parents
when fathers are both supportive partners and nurturing parents. And children
are major beneficiaries when both parents are warm and loving. A second salutary effect of widespread recognition of the influence of
fathers and their love should be to help reduce the incidence of "mother
blaming" common in the clinical field. That is, according to Phares
(1997) and others ( Caplan,
1986 , 1989
; Caplan
& Hall-McCorquodale, 1985 ; Phares
& Compas, 1992 ), the great emphasis on mothers and mothering in
clinical research and practice–without examining the influence of fathers and
fathering–has led to a tendency toward blaming mothers for children's
maladjustment and psychopathology. The emphasis in this review on fathers should not be construed to suggest
that mother love is generally less important than father love. Indeed, some
studies discussing the effects of father love relative to mother love conclude
that mother love has greater influence than father love for specific
developmental outcomes. For example, in their meta-analysis of 15 studies
dealing with the relation between gender of parent and youths' externalizing
behaviors, Rothbaum
and Weisz (1994) found that the correlation between caregiving and
externalizing was larger–but apparently not statistically so–for mothers than
for fathers in 80% of the studies reviewed. According to a related meta-analysis
of the relation between caregiving and conduct problems—delinquency by Loeber
and Stouthamer-Loeber (1986) , however, 64% of the 75 studies reviewed
reported that fathers' love-related behaviors had a stronger association with
these child outcomes than did mothers' behavior. Evidence such as this punctuates the need to include both fathers (and other
significant males, when appropriate) and mothers in future research and then to
analyze the data for possible father and mother effects separately ( K. N.
Harris et al., 1998 ). It is only by separating data in this way that
behavioral scientists can discern when and under what conditions paternal and
maternal factors have similar or different effects on specific child outcomes (
Cole
& McPherson, 1993 ; Kim
& Rohner, 2001 ; Phares,
1996 ; Phares
& Compas, 1992 ). This call for separate measurement and analyses of
maternal and paternal influences is contrary to the argument made by some ( Kurdek
& Fine, 1994 ; Kurdek
& Sinclair, 1988 ; Schwartz,
Barton-Henry, & Pruzinsky, 1985 ) that behavioral scientists should
combine mothers' behavior and fathers' behavior into a single, composite index.
Beyond this, evidence cited in this article makes even more compelling the
call by Silverstein
and Phares (1996) and others (e.g., Jessor,
1993 ; Kerr
& Bowen, 1988 ; Parke,
1995 ; Phares,
1996 ) to "transform the dominant theoretical paradigm in the social
sciences from a dyadic [i.e., mother—child] to a triadic [i.e.,
father—mother—child], or larger systems model" ( Silverstein
& Phares, 1996 , p. 48). This transformation would include a paradigm
shift in guidelines for conducting research as well as in graduate training and
clinical programs to automatically include fathers as well as mothers in all
parenting matters. In addition, this call recognizes the need to explore social
policy implications of research showing the powerful influence of father love as
well as mother love. Finally, we should note that many questions remain unresolved and even
unexplored in the research literature assessing the relative contributions of
mothers' and fathers' love and love withdrawal. For example, little is known
about the question of whether children might be differentially affected by
mother love in comparison with father love at different ages or at different
developmental stages. Similarly, it is unclear whether the magnitude of the
effect of mother love or father love varies across offsprings' life span. And,
as already mentioned, little is yet known about why the influence of father love
is sometimes greater than the influence of mother love. Answers to these and
many other such questions await future research. In this article, the term parents is defined as whoever the most
important caregivers are of a child. These people are not necessarily mothers or
fathers. But typically they are. It would be misleading to imply that all studies of boys' gender role
development dealt with father—son relationships only. The mother—son
relationship was sometimes included. Overall, however, results suggested that a
nurturant father—son relationship is more important to a boy's development of
masculinity than is the mother—son relationship ( Biller
& Borstelmann, 1967 ).
References
Allen, S. M. & Hawkins, A. J.
(1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Mother beliefs and behaviors that inhibit greater
father involvement in family work. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
61, 199-212.
Amato, P. R. (1994). Father-child relations, mother-child relations and
offspring psychological well-being in adulthood. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 56, 1031-1042.
Amato, P. R. (1998). More than money?
Men's contributions to their children's lives.(In A. Booth & A. Crouter
(Eds.), Men in families (pp. 241—278). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.)
Amato, P. R. & Gilbreth, P. R. (1999). Nonresident fathers and
children's well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
61, 557-574.
Amato, P. R. & Rivera, F. (1999). Paternal involvement and
children's behavior problems. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61,
375-384.
Andry, R. G. (1962). Paternal and maternal roles and
delinquency.(In M. Ainsworth (Ed.), Deprivation of maternal care (WHO
Public Papers, Vol. 14, pp. 31—41). Geneva: World Health Organization.)
Arrindell, W., Emmelkamp, P., Monsma, A. & Brilman, E. (1983). The
role of perceived parental rearing practices in the etiology of phobic
disorders: A controlled study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143,
183-187.
Atkinson, M. P. & Blackwelder, S. P. (1993). Fathering in the 20th
century. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 975-986.
Barber, B. & Thomas, D. (1986). Dimensions of fathers' and mothers'
supportive behavior: A case for physical affection. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 48, 783-794.
Barnes, G.
(1984). Adolescent alcohol abuse and other problem behaviors: Their
relationships and common parental influences. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 13, 329-348.
Barnett, R.
C., Marshall, N. L. & Pleck, J. H. (1992). Adult son-parent relationships
and the associations with son's psychological distress. Journal of Family
Issues, 13, 505-525.
Baron, R.
& Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
Barrera, M.
& Garrison-Jones, C. (1992). Family and peer social support as specific
correlates of adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 20, 1-16.
Bartle, S.,
Anderson, S. & Sabatelli, R. (1989). A model of parenting style, adolescent
individuation and adolescent self-esteem: Preliminary findings. Journal of
Adolescent Research, 4, 283-298.
Becker, W. C.
(1960). The relationship of factors in parental ratings of self and each other
to the behavior of kindergarten children as rated by mothers, fathers, and
teachers. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 507-527.
Becker, W. C.
(1964). Consequences of different kinds of parental discipline.(In M. L. Hoffman
& L. W. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child development research (pp.
169—208). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.)
Becker, W. C.,
Peterson, D. R., Hellmer, L. A., Shoemaker, D. J. & Quay, H. C. (1959).
Factors in parental behavior and personality as related to problem behavior in
children. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 23, 107-118.
Belsky, J.
(1998). Paternal influence and children's well-being: Limits of, and new
directions for, understanding.(In A. Booth & A. Crouter (Eds.), Men in
families (pp. 279—293). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.)
Benson, L.
(1968). Fatherhood: A sociological perspective. (New York: Random House)
Biller, H. B. (1974). Paternal deprivation: Family, school,
sexuality, and society. (Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath)
Biller,
H. B. (1981). Father absence, divorce, and personality development.(In M. E.
Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (pp. 489—552).
New York: Wiley.)
Biller, H. B. (1993). Fathers and families:
Paternal factors in child development. (Westport, CT: Auburn House)
Biller, H. B. & Borstelmann, L. J. (1967). Masculine development:
An integrative review. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 13, 253-294.
Booth, A.
& Amato, P. R. (1994). Parental marital quality, parental divorce, and
relations with parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 21-34.
Booth, A. & Crouter, A. C. (1998). Men in families: When
do they get involved? what difference does it make? (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum)
Brody, G. H., Moore, K. & Glei, D. (1994). Family processes
during adolescence as predictors of parents-young adult attitude similarity: A
six-year longitudinal analysis. Family Relations, 43, 369-373.
Bronson, W. C. (1959). Dimensions of ego and infantile identification.
Journal of Personality, 27, 532-545.
Bronstein, P. &
Cowan, C. P. (Eds.) (1988). Fatherhood today: Men's changing role in the
family. (New York: Wiley)
Brook, J. E. & Brook, J. S.
(1988). A developmental approach examining social and personal correlates in
relation to alcohol use over time. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 149,
93-110.
Brook, J. S., Whiteman, M. & Gordon, A. S. (1981). The role of the
father in his son's marijuana use. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138,
81-86.
Buri, J. R. (1989). Self-esteem and appraisals of parental behavior.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 4, 33-49.
Buri, J. R.,
Louiselle, P. A., Misukanis, T. M. & Mueller, R. A. (1988). Effects of
parental authoritarianism and authoritativeness on self-esteem. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 271-282.
Buri, J. R.,
Murphy, P., Richtsmeier, L. M. & Komar, K. K. (1992). Stability of parental
nurturance as a salient predictor of self-esteem. Psychological Reports,
71, 535-543.
Cabrera, N. J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bradley, R. H., Hofferth, S.
& Lamb, M. E. (2000). Fatherhood in the twenty-first century. Child
Development, 71, 127-136.
Campbell, O.
T. & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the
multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 2, 31-75.
Campo, A. T.
& Rohner, R. P. (1992). Relationships between perceived parental
acceptance-rejection, psychological adjustment, and substance abuse among young
adults. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 429-440.
Caplan, P. J.
(1986, October). Take the blame off mother. Psychology Today, , 70-71.
Caplan, P. J.
(1989). Don't blame mother: Mending the mother-daughter relationship.
(New York: Harper & Row)
Caplan, P. J. &
Hall-McCorquodale, I. (1985). The scapegoating of mothers: A call for change.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55, 610-613.
Carroll, A. D.
(1973). Parent acceptance, self-concept, and achievement of kindergarten
children. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Auburn University, Auburn, AL)
Chen, X., Liu, M. & Li, D. (2000). Parental warmth, control,
and indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: A
longitudinal study. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 401-419.
Child Trends.
(2000). Indicators of children's well-being. In Trends in the well-being of
America's children & youth [On-line]. Retrieved from
http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/00trends/contents.htm#PF
Cole, D. &
McPherson, A. E. (1993). Relation of family subsystems to adolescent depression:
Implementing a new family assessment strategy. Journal of Family Psychology,
7, 119-133.
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M.
& Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for
nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218-232.
Collins, W.
A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M. & Bornstein, M. H.
(2001). Toward nature with nurture. American Psychologist, 56, 171-172.
Collins, W. A.
& Russell, G. (1991). Mother-child and father-child relationships in middle
childhood and adolescence: A developmental analysis.(Developmental Review II
, 99-136.)
Cowan, C. P. & Bronstein, P. (1988). Fathers'
roles in the family: Implications for research, intervention, and change.(In P.
Bronstein & C. P. Cowan (Eds.), Fatherhood today: Men's changing role in
the family (pp. 341—347). New York: Wiley.)
Crook, T.,
Raskin, A. & Eliot, J. (1981). Parent-child relationships and adult
depression. Child Development, 52, 950-957.
DeKlyen, M.,
Biernbaum, M., Speltz, M. & Greenberg, M. (1998). Fathers and preschool
behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 34, 264-275.
DeKlyen, M.,
Speltz, M. & Greenberg, M. (1998). Fathering and early onset conduct
problems: Positive and negative parenting, father-son attachment, and the
marital context. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 3-21.
Dekovic, M.
& Meeus, W. (1997). Peer relations in adolescence: Effects of parenting and
adolescents' self-concept. Journal of Adolescence, 20, 163-176.
Dickens, C.
(1843). A Christmas carol. (London: Chapman & Hall)
Dickie, J., Eshleman, A., Merasco, D., Shepard, A., Vander Wilt, M.
& Johnson, M. (1997). Parent-child relationships and children's images of
God. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 36, 25-43.
Distler, L. S.
(1965). Patterns of parental identification: An examination of three
theories. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California,
Berkeley)
Doherty, W. J., Kouneski, E. F. & Erickson, M. F.
(1998). Responsible fathering: An overview and conceptual framework. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 60, 277-292.
Dominy, N. L.,
Johnson, W. B. & Koch, C. (2000). Perception of parental acceptance in women
with binge eating disorder. Journal of Psychology, 134, 23-36.
DuBos, D., Eitel, S. & Felner, R. (1994). Effects of family
environment and parent-child relationships on school adjustment during the
transition to early adolescence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56,
405-414.
Duck, S. (1988). Relating to others. (Chicago:
Dorsey Press)
Duck, S. (1991). Understanding relationships.
(New York: Guilford Press)
Easterbrooks, M. & Goldberg,
W. (1984). Toddler development in the family: Impact of father involvement and
parenting characteristics. Child Development, 55, 740-752.
Eldred, C.,
Brown, Z. & Mahabir, C. (1974). Heroin addict clients' description of their
families of origin. International Journal of Addictions, 9, 315-320.
Ellner, J. (1973). Recent changes in American child rearing
practices: 1950 through 1970. (Unpublished manuscript, Center for the Study
of Parental Acceptance and Rejection, University of Connecticut, Storrs)
Emmelkamp, P. & Heeres, H. (1988). Drug addiction and parental
rearing style: A controlled study. International Journal of Addictions,
23, 207-216.
Emmelkamp, P. & Karsdorp, E. (1987). The effects of perceived
parental rearing style on the development of Type A pattern. European Journal
of Personality, 1, 223-230.
Eron, L. D.,
Banta, T. J., Walder, L. O. & Laulicht, J. H. (1961). Comparison of data
obtained from mothers and fathers on child-rearing practices and their relation
to child aggression. Child Development, 32, 457-472.
Fagot, B. I.
(1995). Parenting boys and girls.(In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 163—183). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.)
Ferholt, J. & Gurwitt, A. (1982). Involving fathers in
treatment.(In S. Cath, A. Gurwitt, & J. Munder Ross (Eds.), Father and
child (pp. 557-568). Boston: Little, Brown.)
Fine, M.,
Voydanoff, P. & Donnelly, B. (1993). Relations between parental control and
warmth and child well-being in stepfamilies. Journal of Family Psychology,
2, 222-232.
Fish, K. & Biller, H. (1973). Perceived childhood paternal
relationships and college females' personal adjustment. Adolescence, 8,
415-420.
Fitz-Simmons, M. J. (1935). Some parent-child relationships as shown
in clinical case studies ((Contributions to Education No. 643). New York:
Teachers College, Columbia University)
Forehand, R. &
Nousiainen, S. (1993). Maternal and paternal parenting: Critical dimensions in
adolescent functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 7, 213-221.
Fox, N. A.,
Kimmerly, N. L. & Schafer, W. D. (1991). Attachment to mother/attachment to
father: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 62, 210-225.
Giveans, D. L.
& Robinson, M. K. (1985). Fathers and the preschool-age child.(In S. M. H.
Hanson & F. W. Bozett (Eds.), Dimensions of fatherhood (pp. 115—140).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.)
Grant, K., O'Koon, J., Davis, T.,
Roache, N., Poindexter, L., Armstrong, M., Minden, J. & McIntosh, J. (2000).
Protective factors affecting low-income urban African American youth exposed to
stress. Journal of Early Adolescence, 20, 388-418.
Green, E.
(1982). Role of the father in female athletic achievement. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 43, 10B
Greenberger, E. & Chen,
C. (1996). Perceived family relationships and depressed mood in early and late
adolescence: A comparison of European and Asian Americans. Developmental
Psychology, 32, 707-716.
Griswold, R.
L. (1993). Fatherhood in America: A history. (New York: Basic Books)
Hall, E. (1849). A mother's influence. Mother's Assistant,
14, 25-29.
Hanson, S. M. & Bozett, F. W. (Eds.) (1985).
Dimensions of fatherhood. (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage)
Hanson, S. M. & Bozett, F. W. (Eds.) (1991). Fatherhood and
families in cultural context. (New York: Springer)
Harris,
J. R. (1998). The nurture assumptions: Why children turn out the way they do.
(New York: Free Press)
Harris, K. N., Furstenberg, F. F.
& Marmer, J. K. (1998). Paternal involvement with adolescents in intact
families: The influence of fathers over the life course. Demography, 35,
201-216.
Heilbrun, A. B., Orr, H. K. & Harrell, S. N.
(1966). Patterns of parental child rearing and subsequent vulnerability to
cognitive disturbance. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 30, 51-59.
Hewlett, B. S.
(Ed.) (1992). Father-child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts.
(New York: Aldine de Gruyter)
Hull, J. G., Tedlie, J. C.
& Zahn, D. A. (1992). Moderator variables in personality research: The
problem of controlling for plausible alternatives. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 18, 115-117.
Huttenen, J.
(1992). Father's impact on son's gender role identity. Scandinavian Journal
of Educational Research, 36, 251-260.
Jacobs, M. A.,
Spilken, A. & Norman, M. (1972). Perception of faulty parent-child
relationships and illness behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 39, 49-55.
Jain, A., Belsky, J. & Crnic, K.
(1996). Beyond fathering: Types of dads. Journal of Family Psychology,
10, 431-442.
Jessor, R. (1993). Successful adolescent development among youth in
high-risk settings. American Psychologist, 48, 117-126.
Jordan, B.,
Radin, N. & Epstein, A. (1975). Paternal behavior and intellectual
functioning in preschool boys and girls. Developmental Psychology, 11,
407-408.
Kagan, J. (1978, August). The parental love trap. Psychology Today,
, 58 61-91.
Kaplan, A. (1964). The conduct of inquiry:
Methodology for behavioral science.(Scranton, PA: Chandler)
Kelly, J. W. & Worell, L. (1976). Parent behavior related to
masculine, feminine, and androgynous sex role orientations. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 843-851.
Kerr, M. E.
& Bowen, M. (1988). Family evaluation. (New York: Norton)
Khaleque, A. & Rohner, R. P. (in press). Perceived parental
acceptance-rejection and psychological adjustment: A meta-analysis of
cross-cultural and intracultural studies. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
,
Kim, K. & Rohner, R. P. (2001). Parental warmth,
control, and involvement in schooling: Predicting academic achievement among
Korean American adolescents. (Manuscript submitted for publication)
Komarovsky, M. (1976). Dilemma of masculinity: A study of college
youth. (New York: Norton)
Kroupa, S. (1988). Perceived
parental acceptance and female juvenile delinquency. Adolescence, 23,
171-185.
Kurdek, L. A. & Fine, M. A. (1994). Family acceptance and family
control as predictors of adjustment in young adolescents: Linear, curvilinear,
or interactive effects? Child Development, 65, 1137-1146.
Kurdek, L. A.
& Sinclair, R. J. (1988). Adjustment of young adolescents in two-parent
nuclear, stepfather, and mother-custody families. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 56, 91-96.
Lamb, M. E.
(1975). Fathers: Forgotten contributors to child development. Human
Development, 18, 245-266.
Lamb, M. E.
(Ed.) (1981). The role of the father in child development ((2nd ed.). New
York: Wiley)
Lamb, M. E. (1986). The changing role of
fathers.(In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The father's role: Applied perspectives
(pp. 3—28). New York: Wiley.)
Lamb, M. E. (1997). Fathers
and child development: An introductory overview and guide.(In M. E. Lamb (Ed.),
The role of the father in child development , (pp. 1-18). New York:
Wiley.)
Lamb, M. E. (2000). The history of research on father
involvement: An overview. Marriage and Family Review, 29, 23-42.
Lamb, M. E.,
Pleck, J. H., Charnov, E. L. & Levine, J. A. (1987). A biosocial perspective
on paternal behavior and involvement.(In J. B. Lancaster, J. Altmann, A. S.
Rossi, & L. R. Sherrod (Eds.), Parenting across the lifespan: Biosocial
dimensions (pp. 111—142). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.)
La
Rossa, R. (1997). The modernization of fatherhood: A social and political
history. (Chicago: University of Chicago Press)
La Rossa,
R., Jaret, E., Gadgil, M. & Wynn, G. R. (2000). The changing culture of
fatherhood in comic-strip families: A six-decade analysis. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 62, 375-387.
Lee, H. (1960). To
kill a mockingbird. (New York: J. B. Lippincott)
Lefkowitz,
M. & Tesiny, E. (1984). Rejection and depression: Prospective and
contemporaneous analyses. Developmental Psychology, 20, 776-785.
Lidz, R. W.
& Lidz, T. (1949). The family environment of the schizophrenic patient.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 106, 332-345.
Lidz, T.,
Parker, B. & Cornelison, A. (1956). The role of the father in the family
environment of the schizophrenic patient. American Journal of Psychiatry,
113, 126-132.
Loeber, R. & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1986). Family factors as
correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency.(In M.
Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.), Crime and justice (Vol. 7, pp. 29—149).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.)
Lupton, D. &
Barclay, L. (1997). Constructing Fatherhood: Discourses and experiences.(London:
Sage)
Maccoby, E. E. & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization
in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction.(In E. M. Hetherington
(Ed.), Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, 4th
ed., pp. 1—101). New York: Wiley.)
Mackey, W. C. (1996). The
American father: Biocultural and developmental aspects. (New York: Plenum)
Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R. P. & Lamb, M. E. (2000).
Scholarship on fatherhood in the 1990s and beyond. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 62, 1173-1191.
Matsuda, K.
& Ritblatt, S. (1998, November). The impact of family parenting styles on
the separation-individuation process among adult only-children. (Paper
presented at the 60th Annual Conference of the National Council on Family
Relations, Milwaukee, WI)
McPherson, S. R. (1974). Parental
interactions of various levels. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
158, 424-431.
Mead, M. (1956). Some theoretical considerations on the problem of
mother-child separation.(In D. G. Haring (Ed.), Personal character and
cultural milieu (pp. 637—649). Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.)
Millen, L. & Roll, S. (1997). Relationships between sons'
feelings of being understood by their fathers and measures of the sons'
psychological functioning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 130, 19-25.
Monkman, J.
A. (1958). The relationship between children's adjustment and parental
acceptance. Dissertation Abstracts, 19, 1117-1118.
Mussen, P. H.
(1961). Some antecedents and consequences of masculine sex-typing in adolescent
boys. Psychological Monographs, ,
Mussen, P. H. &
Distler, L. (1959). Masculinity, identification, and father-son relationships.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 350-356.
Musser, J.
& Fleck, J. (1983). The relationship of paternal acceptance and control to
college females' personality adjustment. Adolescence, 18, 907-916.
Nash, J.
(1965). The father in contemporary culture and current psychological literature.
Child Development, 36, 261-291.
Orlofsky, J.
L. (1979). Parental antecedents of sex-role orientation in college men and
women. Sex Roles, 5, 495-512.
Paley, B.,
Conger, R. & Harold, G. (2000). Parents' affect, adolescent cognitive
representations, and adolescent social development. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 62, 761-776.
Parke, R. D.
(1985). Foreword.(In S. M. H. Hanson & F. W. Bozett (Eds.), Dimensions of
fatherhood (pp. 9-12). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.)
Parke, R.
D. (1995). Fathers and families.(In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp. 27-63).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.)
Parke, R. D. (1996). Fatherhood.
(Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press)
Parsons, T. &
Bales, R. F. (Eds.) (1955). Family, socialization, and interaction process.
(New York: Free Press)
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B. &
Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial boys. (Eugene, OR: Castalia)
Payne, D. E. & Mussen, P. H. (1956). Parent child relations and
father identification among adolescent boys. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 52, 358-362.
Peppin, B. H.
(1962). Parental understanding, parental acceptance, and the self concept of
children as a function of academic over and under achievement. Dissertation
Abstracts, 23, 4422-4423.
Peterson, D.
R., Becker, W. C., Hellmer, L. A., Shoemaker, D. J. & Quay, H. C. (1959).
Parental attitudes and child adjustment. Child Development, 30,
119-130.
Peterson, D. R., Becker, W. C., Shoemaker, D., Luria, Z. &
Hellmer, L. A. (1961). Child behavior problems and parental attitudes. Child
Development, 32, 151-162.
Phares, V.
(1992). Where's Poppa? The relative lack of attention to the role of fathers in
child and adolescent psychopathology. American Psychologist, 47,
656-666.
Phares, V. (1996). Fathers and Developmental Psychology. (New
York: Wiley)
Phares, V. (1997). Psychological adjustment,
maladjustment, and father-child relationships.(In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role
of the father in child development (3rd ed., pp. 261-283). New York: Wiley.)
Phares, V. & Compas, B. E. (1992). The role of fathers in
child and adolescent psychopathology: Make room for Daddy. Psychological
Bulletin, 111, 387-412.
Pleck, E. H.
& Pleck, J. H. (1997). Fatherhood ideals in the United States: Historical
dimensions.(In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development
(3rd ed., pp. 33-48). New York: Wiley.)
Pleck, J. H.
(1997). Paternal involvement: Level, sources, and consequences.(In M. E. Lamb
(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (3rd ed., pp. 66—103).
New York: Wiley.)
Prendergast, T. & Schaefer, E. (1974).
Correlates of drinking and drunkenness among high school students. Quarterly
Journal in the Study of Alcoholism, 35, 232-242.
Radin, N.
(1981). The role of the father in cognitive/academic and intellectual
development.(In M. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development
(2nd ed., pp. 379—427). New York: Wiley.)
Radin, N. &
Russell, G. (1983). Increased father participation and child development
outcomes.(In M. Lamb & A. Sagi (Eds.), Fatherhood and family policy
(pp. 191-218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.)
Radin, N. &
Sagi, A. (1982). Childrearing fathers in intact families in Israel and the
U.S.A. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 111-136.
Radin, N.,
Williams, E. & Coggins, K. (1993, October). Paternal involvement in
childrearing and the school performance of Native American children: An
exploratory study. (Paper presented at the Conference on Race/Ethnic
Families in the U. S., Provo, UT)
Rapoport, R., Rapoport, R.
N., Strelitz, Z. & Kew, S. (1977). Fathers, mothers, and society.
(New York: Basic Books)
Renk, K., Phares, V. & Epps, J.
(1999). The relationship between parental anger and behavior problems in
children and adolescents. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 209-227.
Reuter, M.
& Biller, H. (1973). Perceived nurturance, availability, and personality
adjustment of college males. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
40, 339-342.
Richter, J., Richter, G. & Eisemann, E. (1990). Parental rearing
behaviour, family atmosphere, and adult depression: A pilot study with
psychiatric inpatients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 82, 219-222.
Rohner, R. P.
(1960). Child acceptance-rejection and modal personality in three Pacific
societies. (Unpublished master's thesis, Stanford University, Stanford, CA)
Rohner, R. P. (1975). They love me, they love me not: A
worldwide study of the effects of parental acceptance and rejection. (New
Haven, CT: HRAF Press)
Rohner, R. P. (1986). The warmth
dimension: Foundations of parental acceptance-rejection theory. (Newbury
Park, CA: Sage)
Rohner, R. P. (1994). Patterns of parenting:
The warmth dimension in worldwide perspective.(In W. J. Lonner & R. S.
Malpass (Eds.), Readings in psychology and culture (pp. 113-120). Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.)
Rohner, R. P. (1995).
[Semantic analysis of students' conceptions of "fathering" versus
"mothering"].(Unpublished raw data)
Rohner, R. P. (1998).
Father love and child development: History and current evidence. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 157-161.
Rohner, R. P.
(1999). Acceptance and rejection.(In D. Levinson, J. Ponzetti, & P.
Jorgensen (Eds.), Encyclopedia of human emotions (Vol. 1, pp. 6—14). New
York: Macmillan Reference.)
Rohner, R. P. (2001). Parental
acceptance and rejection bibliography ([On-line]. Retrieved from
http://vm.uconn.edu/~rohner/CSPARBL.htm)
Rohner, R. P. &
Britner, P. A. (in press). Worldwide mental health correlates of parental
acceptance-rejection: Review of cross-cultural and intracultural evidence.
Cross-Cultural Research, ,
Rohner, R. & Brothers, S.
(1999). Perceived parental rejection, psychological maladjustment, and
borderline personality disorder. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 1, 81-95.
Rohner, R. P. & Nielsen, C. C. (1978). Parental
acceptance and rejection: A review and annotated bibliography of research and
theory. (New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files)
Rohner,
R. P. & Rohner, E. C. (1981). Parental acceptance-rejection and parental
control: Cross-cultural codes. Ethnology, 20, 245-260.
Rohner, R. P. & Rohner, E. C. (1982). Enculturative continuity and
the importance of caregivers: Cross-cultural codes. Behavior Science
Research, 17, 91-114.
Rothbaum, F.
& Weisz, J. R. (1994). Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior
in nonclinical samples: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116,
55-74.
Rowe, D. C. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes,
experience and behavior. (New York: Guilford Press)
Rowe,
D. C. (2001). The nurture assumption persists. American Psychologist, 56,
168-169.
Russell, A. & Russell, G. (1996). Positive parenting and boys' and
girls' misbehaviour during a home observation. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 19, 291-307.
Schwartz, J.
C., Barton-Henry, M. & Pruzinsky, T. (1985). Assessing child-rearing
behaviors: A comparison of ratings made by mother, father, child, and sibling on
the CRPBI. Child Development, 56, 462-479.
Sears, R. R.
(1970). Relation of early socialization experience to self-concepts and gender
role in middle childhood. Child Development, 41, 267-289.
Siantz, M.
& Smith, M. (1994). Parental factors correlated with developmental outcome
in the migrant Head Start child. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9,
481-503.
Silverstein, L. B. & Auerbach, C. F. (1999). Deconstructing the
essential father. American Psychologist, 54, 397-407.
Silverstein,
L. B. & Phares, V. (1996). Expanding the mother-child paradigm: An
examination of dissertation research, 1986-1994. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 20, 39-53.
Simons, R.
L., Whitbeck, L. B., Conger, R. D. & Melby, J. N. (1990). Husband and wife
differences in determinants of parenting: A social learning and exchange model
of parental behavior. Journal of Marriage and Family, 52, 375-392.
Stagner, R. (1933). The role of parents in the development of
emotional instability. Psychological Bulletin, 30, 696-697.
Stearns, P.
N. (1991). Fatherhood in historical perspective: The role of social change.(In
F. W. Bozett & S. M. H. Hanson (Eds.), Fatherhood and family in cultural
context (pp. 28—52). New York: Springer.)
Stendler, C. B.
(1950). Sixty years of child training practices. Journal of Pediatrics,
36, 122-134.
Sunley, R. (1955). Early nineteenth-century American literature on
child rearing.(In M. Mead & M. Wolfenstein (Eds.), Childhood in
contemporary cultures (pp. 150—167). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.)
Tacon, A. & Caldera, Y. (2001). Attachment and parental
correlates in late adolescent Mexican American women. Hispanic Journal of
Behavioral Sciences, 23, 71-88.
Terman, L. M.
& Merrill, M. A. (1960). The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale.
(Boston: Houghton Mifflin)
Veneziano, R. (1998, February).
The influence of paternal warmth and involvement on offspring behavior:
Holocultural evidence. (Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for
Cross-Cultural Research, Tampa, FL)
Veneziano, R. (2000).
Perceived paternal and maternal warmth and African American and European
American youths' psychological adjustment. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 62, 123-132.
Veneziano, R.
& Rohner, R. (1998). Perceived paternal acceptance, paternal involvement,
and youths' psychological adjustment in a rural, biracial southern community.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 335-343.
Wagner, B.
& Philips, D. (1992). Beyond beliefs: Parent and child behaviors and
children's perceived academic competence. Child Development, 63,
1380-1391.
Walters, J. & Stinnett, N. (1971). Parent-child relationships: A
decade review of research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 33,
70-111.
Williams, E. & Radin, N. (1993). Paternal
involvement, maternal employment, and adolescents' academic achievement: An
11-year follow-up. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 63, 306-312.
Williams, S.
& Finley, G. (1997). Father contact and perceived affective quality of
fathering in Trinidad. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 31, 315-319.
Yamasaki, K. (1990). Parental child-rearing attitudes
associated with type A behaviors in children. Psychological Reports, 67,
235-239.
Young, M. H., Miller, B. E., Norton, M. C. & Hill, J. E. (1995).
The effect of parental supportive behaviors on life satisfaction of adolescent
offspring. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 813-822.
1
2
Correspondence may be addressed to
Electronic mail may be sent to Rohner@uconn.edu
Received:
Revised: April 10, 2001
Accepted: April 10,
2001